K. Hirsh-pasek and R. Treiman, Motherese in a new context, J. Child Lang, vol.9, pp.229-23710, 1982.

D. Burnham, Are you my little pussy-cat? Acoustic, phonetic and affective qualities of infant-and pet-directed speech, ICSLP, 1998.

D. Burnham, C. Kitamura, and U. Vollmer-conna, What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals, Science, vol.296, issue.5572, pp.1435-143510, 2002.
DOI : 10.1126/science.1069587

R. W. Mitchell, Americans' Talk to Dogs: Similarities and Differences With Talk to Infants, Research on Language & Social Interaction, vol.41, issue.2, pp.183-21010, 2001.
DOI : 10.1037//0012-1649.24.1.14

R. W. Mitchell and E. Edmonson, Functions of Repetitive Talk to Dogs during Play: Control, Conversation, or Planning?, Society & Animals, vol.7, issue.1, pp.55-81, 1999.
DOI : 10.1163/156853099X00167

L. Singh, J. L. Morgan, and C. T. Best, Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk?, Infancy, vol.43, issue.3, pp.365-39410, 2002.
DOI : 10.1037/h0084224

URL : http://titan.cog.brown.edu/research/infant_lab/baby talk or happy talk.pdf

A. Fernald, Intonation and Communicative Intent in Mothers' Speech to Infants: Is the Melody the Message?, Child Development, vol.60, issue.6, pp.1497-151010, 1989.
DOI : 10.2307/1130938

L. J. Trainor, C. M. Austin, and R. N. Desjardin, Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion?, Psychological Science, vol.31, issue.3, pp.188-9510, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/0163-6383(94)90012-4

S. Jeannin, C. Gilbert, and G. Leboucher, Effect of interaction type on the characteristics of pet-directed speech in female dog owners. Anim Cog., Available online, pp.10-1007, 2017.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01478437

A. Fernald, Intonation and Communicative Intent in Mothers' Speech to Infants: Is the Melody the Message?, Child Development, vol.60, issue.6, pp.1497-1510, 1989.
DOI : 10.2307/1130938

N. Naoi, Cerebral responses to infant-directed speech and the effect of talker familiarity, NeuroImage, vol.59, issue.2, pp.1735-1744, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.093

C. Saint-georges, Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review), PLoS ONE, vol.8, issue.10, 2013.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.s002

R. M. Golinkoff, D. D. Can, M. Soderstrom, and K. Hirsh-pasek, (Baby)Talk to Me, Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol.24, issue.5, pp.339-34410, 2015.
DOI : 10.1207/s15327078in1101_2

W. Ma, R. M. Golinkoff, D. Houston, and K. Hirsh-pasek, Word Learning in Infant- and Adult-Directed Speech, Language Learning and Development, vol.11, issue.3, pp.209-225579839, 2011.
DOI : 10.1207/s15327078in1101_2

URL : https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5679190/pdf

J. Y. Song, K. Demuth, and J. Morgan, Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol.128, issue.1, pp.389-400, 2010.
DOI : 10.1121/1.3419786

N. Xu, D. Burnham, C. Kitamura, and U. Vollmer-conna, Vowel Hyperarticulation in Parrot-, Dog- and Infant-Directed Speech, Anthrozo??s, vol.491, issue.3, pp.373-38010, 2013.
DOI : 10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003

G. W. Mcroberts, C. Mcdonough, and L. Lakusta, The Role of Verbal Repetition in the Development of Infant Speech Preferences From 4 to 14 Months of Age, Infancy, vol.14, issue.2, pp.162-1941015250000802707062, 1080.
DOI : 10.1080/15250000802707062

C. Dunst, E. Gorman, and D. Hamby, Preference for infant-directed speech in preverbal young children, CELL, vol.5, pp.1-13, 2012.

A. Schachner and E. E. Hannon, Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants., Developmental Psychology, vol.47, issue.1, pp.19-25, 2011.
DOI : 10.1037/a0020740

R. Zangl and D. L. Mills, Increased Brain Activity to Infant-Directed Speech in 6- and 13-Month-Old Infants, Infancy, vol.17, issue.1, pp.31-6210, 2007.
DOI : 10.1037/h0084224

M. Nagasawa, Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds, Science, vol.64, issue.2, pp.333-33610, 2015.
DOI : 10.1210/jcem-64-2-340

L. E. Stoeckel, L. S. Palley, R. L. Gollub, S. M. Niemi, and A. Evins, Patterns of Brain Activation when Mothers View Their Own Child and Dog: An fMRI Study, PLoS ONE, vol.104, issue.10, 2014.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0107205.t005

A. Andics, M. Gácsi, T. Faragó, A. Kis, and Á. Miklósi, Voice-Sensitive Regions in the Dog and Human Brain Are Revealed by Comparative fMRI, Current Biology, vol.24, issue.5, pp.574-578, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.058

P. Pongrácz, Á. Miklósi, K. Timár-geng, and V. Csányi, Verbal Attention Getting as a Key Factor in Social Learning Between Dog (Canis familiaris) and Human., Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol.118, issue.4, pp.375-383, 2004.
DOI : 10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.375

E. Téglás, A. Gergely, K. Kupán, Á. Miklósi, and J. Topál, Dogs' Gaze Following Is Tuned to Human Communicative Signals, Current Biology, vol.22, issue.3, pp.209-212, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.018

L. Scheider, S. Grassmann, J. Kaminski, and M. Tomasello, Domestic Dogs Use Contextual Information and Tone of Voice when following a Human Pointing Gesture, PLoS ONE, vol.116, issue.7, 2011.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0021676.s001

T. Ben-aderet, M. Gallego-abenza, D. Reby, and N. Mathevon, Dog-directed speech: why do we use it and do dogs pay attention to it?, Proc. R Soc B, pp.2016-2429, 2017.
DOI : 10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01441127

P. Mongillo, G. Bono, L. Regolin, and L. Marinelli, Selective attention to humans in companion dogs, Canis familiaris, Animal Behaviour, vol.80, issue.6, pp.1057-1063, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.014

L. Horn, F. Range, and L. Huber, Dogs' attention towards humans depends on their relationship, not only on social familiarity. Anim Cog, pp.435-44310, 2013.
DOI : 10.1007/s10071-012-0584-9

URL : https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4041126/pdf

V. F. Ratcliffe and D. Reby, Orienting Asymmetries in Dogs??? Responses to Different Communicatory Components of Human Speech, Current Biology, vol.24, issue.24, pp.2908-2912030, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.030

A. Andics, Neural mechanisms for lexical processing in dogs, Science, vol.353, issue.6303, pp.1030-103210, 2016.
DOI : 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

C. Darwin, On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for, Life, 1859.

J. J. Ohala, of Voice, Phonetica, vol.41, issue.1, pp.1-16, 1984.
DOI : 10.1159/000261706

E. S. Morton, On the Occurrence and Significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in Some Bird and Mammal Sounds, The American Naturalist, vol.111, issue.981, pp.855-86910, 1977.
DOI : 10.1086/283219

A. M. Taylor, V. F. Ratcliffe, K. Mccomb, and D. Reby, Auditory Communication in Domestic Dogs, pp.131-163, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/B978-0-12-407818-5.00005-X

URL : http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/50634/1/6._Taylor_et_al._The_SOCIAL_DOG_2014.pdf

J. C. Whitham, M. S. Gerald, and D. Maestripieri, Intended Receivers and Functional Significance of Grunt and Girney Vocalizations in Free-Ranging Female Rhesus Macaques, Ethology, vol.138, issue.9, pp.862-874, 2007.
DOI : 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00907.x

X. Chen, J. Yang, S. Gan, and Y. Yang, The Contribution of Sound Intensity in Vocal Emotion Perception: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence, PLoS ONE, vol.134, issue.1, 2012.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0030278.g007

N. Masataka, Pitch modification when interacting with elders: Japanese women with and without experience with infants, Journal of Child Language, vol.29, issue.04, pp.939-95110, 2002.
DOI : 10.1017/S0305000902005378

M. Uther, M. A. Knoll, and D. Burnham, Do you speak E-NG-L-I-SH? A comparison of foreigner- and infant-directed speech, Speech Communication, vol.49, issue.1, pp.2-7, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003

G. Csibra, Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind Lang, pp.141-168, 2010.

J. Topál, A. Kis, and K. Oláh, Dogs??? Sensitivity to Human Ostensive Cues, pp.319-346, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/B978-0-12-407818-5.00011-5

J. A. Serpell, Factors influencing human attitudes to animals and their welfare, Anim welf, vol.13, pp.145-151, 2004.

J. S. Odendaal and R. A. Meintjes, Neurophysiological Correlates of Affiliative Behaviour between Humans and Dogs, The Veterinary Journal, vol.165, issue.3, pp.296-301, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-X

M. Nagasawa, T. Kikusui, T. Onaka, and M. Ohta, Dog's gaze at its owner increases owner's urinary oxytocin during social interaction, Hormones and Behavior, vol.55, issue.3, pp.434-441002, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.12.002

M. H. Yong and T. Ruffman, Domestic dogs match human male voices to faces, but??not for females, Behaviour, vol.110, issue.11, pp.1585-16001568539, 2015.
DOI : 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.018

V. F. Ratcliffe, K. Mccomb, and D. Reby, Cross-modal discrimination of human gender by domestic dogs, Animal Behaviour, vol.91, pp.127-135, 2014.
DOI : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.03.009

J. Call, J. Bräuer, J. Kaminski, and M. Tomasello, Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans., Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol.117, issue.3, pp.257-263, 2003.
DOI : 10.1037/0735-7036.117.3.257

M. Fukuzawa, D. S. Mills, and J. J. Cooper, More than just a word: non-semantic command variables affect obedience in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris), Applied Animal Behaviour Science, vol.91, issue.1-2, pp.129-141025, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.025

P. Boersma and D. Weenink, Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.3.23) See http, 2012.

J. E. Andruski, P. K. Kuhl, and A. Hayashi, Point vowels in Japanese mothers??? speech to infants and adults, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol.105, issue.2, pp.1095-1096, 1999.
DOI : 10.1121/1.425135

S. E. Maxwell and H. D. Delaney, Designing Experiments and Analyzing Data: A Model Comparison Perspective, 2004.